يعرض 1 - 20 نتائج من 28 نتيجة بحث عن '"語誤"', وقت الاستعلام: 0.53s تنقيح النتائج
  1. 1
    Academic Journal
  2. 2
    Academic Journal
  3. 3

    المؤلفون: Hiromi, TSUJI

    المساهمون: 人文科学分野, Humanities, 大阪樟蔭女子大学学芸学部心理学科, Faculty of Liberal Arts, Department of Psychology, Osaka Shoin Women's University

    المصدر: 大阪樟蔭女子大学研究紀要 = Research Bulletin of Osaka Shoin Women's University. 7:27-38

  4. 4

    المؤلفون: Satsuki, Tomita

    المساهمون: 神田外大学留学生別科非常勤;神田外大学言科学研究科

    المصدر: 科学研究 : 神田外大学大学院紀要 = Studies in language sciences. 21:101-110

    مصطلحات موضوعية: 長音, 外来習得, 促音, 外来表記

  5. 5
  6. 6
  7. 7
    Academic Journal

    المؤلفون: J·希利斯·

    المساهمون: 美国哈佛大学, 北京大学,北京,100871

    Relation: 江西社会科学.2007,(1),36-44.; 752940; http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11897/206711

  8. 8
    Report
  9. 9
    Report
  10. 10
  11. 11
    Dissertation/ Thesis
  12. 12
    Dissertation/ Thesis

    المؤلفون: 李冠霆, Lee, Kuan Ting

    المساهمون: 萬依萍, Wan, I Ping

    وصف الملف: 15468405 bytes; application/pdf

    Relation: 吳宗濟(1987)。普通話輔音不送氣/送氣區別的實驗研究。中國言學報,3,256-283。\n鄭靜宜(2011)。音聲學—說話聲音的科學。台北:心理出版社。\nAcheson, D. J., & MacDonald, M. C. (2009). Twisting tongues and memories: Explorations of the relationship between language production and verbal working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 329-350.\nBaars, B. J., Motley, M. T., & MacKay, D. G. (1975). Output editing for lexical status in artificially elicited slips of the tongue. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 382-391.\nBaars, B. J., & MacKay, D. G. (1978). Experimentally eliciting phonetic and sentential speech errors: methods, implications, and work in progress. Language in Society 7(1), 105-109.\nBaars, B. J. (1980). On eliciting predictable speech errors in the laboratory. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in Linguistic Performance: Slips of the Tongue, Ear, Pen, and Hand (pp. 307-318). New York, NY: Academic Press.\nBaars, B. J. (1992). A dozen Competing-Plans techniques for inducing predictable slips in speech and action. In B. J. Baars (Ed.), Experimental Slips and Human Error (pp. 129-150). New York, NY: Plenum Press.\nBerg, T. (1987). A cross-linguistic comparison of slips of the tongue. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club.\nBlumstein, S. E. (1973). A phonological investigation of aphasic speech. The Hague: Mouton.\nChen, K., Tsay, J., & Hong, G. (1998). Duration of initials in Mandarin: Fundamental acoustic research and its clinical significance. Journal of Speech Langauge-hearing Association, 13, 138-149.\nChen, L-M., Chao, K-Y., & Peng, J-F. (2007). VOT productions of word-initial stops in Mandarin and English: A cross-language study. Proceedings of the 19th Conference on Computational Linguistics and Speech Processing, 303-317.\nCho, T., & Ladefoged, P. (1999). Variation and universals in VOT: Evidence from 18 languages. Journal of Phonetics, 27, 207-229.\nEllis, A. W. (1980). Errors in speech and short-term memory: The effects of phonemic similarity and syllable position. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 624-634.\nFellbaum, M. L. (1983). Markedness and allophonic rules. In F. R. Eckman, E. A. Moravcisik & J. R. Wirth (Eds.), Markedness (pp. 291-308). New York, NY: Plenum Press. \nFromkin, V. A. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language, 47(1), 27-52.\nFromkin, V. A. (1973). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Speech errors as linguistic evidence (pp. 215-242). The Hague: Mouton.\nFromkin, V. A., Rodman, R., & Hyams, N. (2007). An Introduction to Language. 8th Edition. Boston, MA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.\nGandour, J. (1977). Counterfeit tones in the speech of southern Thai bidilectals. Lingua, 41, 125–143.\nGoldrick, M., & Blumstein, S. E. (2006). Cascading activation from phonological planning to articulatory processes: Evidence from tongue twisters. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(6), 649-683.\nGormley, A. (2014). Acoustic evidence for phonologically mismatched speech errors. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1-13. \nHowell, P., & Rosen, S. (1983). Production and perception of rise time in the voiceless affricate/fricative distinction. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 73, 976-984.\nHsu, H-L. (2011). The emergence of an unmarkedness effect in Mandarin speech errors: Nasals in a coda position. Language and Speech, 54(3), 307-340.\nHume, E. (2011). Markedness. In M. Van Oostendorp, C. Ewen, E. Hume & K. Rice (Eds), The Blackwell Companion to Phonology (pp.79-106). Blackwell.\nJaeger, J. (2004). Kids’ slips: What young children’s slips of the tongue reveal about language development. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.\nJeng, J-Y. (2005). The influence of speaking rate, utterance unit and position on segmental duration of Mandarin. Journal of National University of Tainan, 39(1), 161-185.\nJohnson, K. (2002) Acoustic and Auditory Phonetics. 2nd Edition. Oxford: Blackwell.\nKeating, P. A. (1984). Phonetic and phonological representation of stop consonant voicing. Language, 60(2), 286-319.\nKent, R. D., & Read, C. (2002). The acoustic analysis of speech. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing.\nLadefoged, P., & Johnson, K. (2011). A course in phonetics. Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.\nLai, Y-H. (2013). VOT for Mandarin stops and affricates produced by L1 and L2 speakers of Mandarin Chinese. Kaohsiung Normal University Journal, 35, 57-78.\nLin, Y. H. (2007). The Sounds of Chinese. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.\nLisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurement. Word, 20, 384-422.\nLiu, H., Ng, M. L., Wan, M., Wang, S., & Zhang, Y. (2007). Effects of place of articulation and aspiration on voice onset time in Mandarin esophageal speech. Folia Phoniatr Logop, 59, 147-154.\nLiu, H-M., Tseng, C-H., & Tsao, F-M. (1999). The speech acoustic characteristic of Mandarin Phonetic Contrasts. Proceedings of the National Science Council (Part C: Humanities and Social Science), 9(4), 726-738.\nMarquardt, T. P., Reinhart, J. B., & Peterson, H. A. (1979). Markedness analysis of phonemic substitution errors in apraxia of speech. Journal of communication disorders, 12(6), 481-494.\nMotley, M. T., & Baars, B. J. (1976). Laboratory induction of verbal slips: A new method for psycholinguistic research. Communication Quarterly, 24(2), 28-34.\nNooteboom, S. G. (1973). The tongue slips into patterns. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Speech errors as linguistic evidence (pp. 144-156). The Hague: Mouton.\nPage, M. P. A., Madge, A., Cumming, N., & Norris, D. G. (2007). Speech errors and the phonological similarity effect in short-term memory: Evidence suggesting a common locus. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 49-64.\nPeng, J-F., Chen, L-M., & Lin, Y-Y. (2009). Tonal effects on voice onset time: Stops in Mandarin and Hakka. Proceedings of the 21st Conference on Computational Linguistics and Speech Processing (ROCLING XXI), 115-123.\nShattuck-Hufnagel, S., & Klatt, D. H. (1979). The limited use of distinctive features and markedness in speech production: evidence from speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 41-55.\nShattuck-Hufnagel, S. (1992). The role of word structure in segmental serial ordering. Cognition, 42, 213-259.\nStemberger, J. P. (1992). The reliability and replicability of naturalistic speech error data: A comparison with experimentally induced errors. In B. J. Baars (Ed.), Experimental Slips and Human Error (pp. 195-215). New York, NY: Plenum Press.\nTse, J. K-P. (1988). A spectrographic analysis of the coronal affricates and fricatives in Mandarin Chinese. Studies in English Literature and Linguistics, 14, 149-199.\nWan, I. P. (2002). Asymmetry in Mandarin consonant articulations: Evidence from slips of the tongue. Concentric: Studies in English Literature and Linguistics, 28(2), 1-25.\nWan, I. P. (2007). On the phonological organization of Mandarin tones. Lingua, 117, 1715-1738.\nWilshire, C. E. (1999). The “tongue twister” paradigm as a technique for studying phonological encoding. Language and Speech, 42(1), 57-82.\nWolk, L. (1986). Markedness analysis of consonant error productions in apraxia of speech. Journal of communication disorders, 19(2), 133-160.\nWu, H-L. (2009). Stops and affricates in Mandarin Chinese and Hakka—A VOT Analysis. MA thesis, Kaohsiung Normal University.\nYiu, H. Y. (2008). Cantonese affricates: an acoustic analysis. BA thesis. The University of Hong Kong.; G0101555008; https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw//handle/140.119/76398; https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/bitstream/140.119/76398/1/500801.pdf

  13. 13
  14. 14
  15. 15
  16. 16
  17. 17
  18. 18
    Dissertation/ Thesis

    المؤلفون: 蕭裕台

    المساهمون: 萬依萍, I-Ping Wan

    Time: 68

    Relation: Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575–589.; Berg, T. (2005). A Structural Account of Phonological Paraphasias. Brain and Language, 94, 104-129.; Boomer, D. S. & Laver, J. D. M. (1968). Slips of the tongue. British Journal of Disorders of Communication 3, 2-12.; Buckingham, H. W. (1980). On correlating aphasic errors with slips-of-tongue. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 199-220.; Butterworth, B. L. (1980). Evidence from pauses in speech. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production (Vol. 1, pp. 155-176). London: Academic Press.; Butterworth, B. L. (1982). Speech errors: Old date in search of new theories. In A. Culter (Ed.), Slips of tongue and language production. Amsterdam: Mouton.; Chen, J.-Y. (1999). The representation and processing of tone in Mandarin Chinese: Evidence from slips of the tongue. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 289-301.; Chen, J.-Y. & Dell, G. S. (2003). Word Form Encoding in Chinese Speech Production. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 45 (4), 313-322.; Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review 93 (3), 283-321.; Dell, G. S. (1988). The retrieval of phonological forms in production: Tests of predictions from a connectionist model. Journal of Memory and Language 27, 124-142.; Dell, G. S.& Gordon, J. K. (2003). Neighbors in the lexicon: Friends or foes? In N. O. Schiller and A. S. Meyer (Eds.), Phonetics and phonology in language comprehension and production: Differences and similarities. New York: Mouton de Grugter.; Dell, G. S.& Juliano, C. (1996). Computational models of phonological encoding. In T. Dijkstra & K. de Smedt (Eds.) Computational psycholinguistics: AI and connectionist models of human language processing (pp. 328-359). Taylor & Francis.; Dell, G. S.& Juliano & Govindjee, A. (1993). Structure and content in language production: A theory of frame constraints in phonological speech errors. Cognitive Science, 17, 149-195.; Dell, G. S. & O’Seaghdha, P. G. (1991). Mediated and convergent lexical priming in language production: A comment on Levelt at al. (1991). Psychological Review.; Dell, G. S. & O’Seaghdha, P. G. (1992). Stages of lexical access in language production. Cognition 42, 287-314.; Dell, G. S. & Reich, P. A. (1981) Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 611-629.; Dell, G. S., & Repka, R. J. (1992). Errors in inner speech. In B. J. Baars (Ed.), Experimental slips and human error: Exploring the architecture of volition, 237–262. New York: Plenum.; Dell, G. S. & Svec, W. R. (1997). Language Production and Serial Order: A Functional Analysis and a Model, Psychological Review, 104 (1), 123-147.; Dell, G. S. & Schwartz, M. F. & Martin, N. & Gagnon, D. A. (1997). Lexical Access in Aphasic and Nonaphasic Speakers. Psychological Review, 104 (4), 801-838.; Dell, G. S., Lawler, E. N., Harris, H. D., & Gordon, J. K. (2004). Models of errors of omission in aphasic naming. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 125–145.; Fay, D. & Cutler, A. (1977). The Phonology of Standard Chinese. Oxford University Press.; Forster, K. I. (1976). Accessing the mental lexicon. In F. J. Wales & E. Walker (Eds.) New Approaches to language mechanisms (pp.257-287). Amsterdam: North-Holland.; Fromkin, V. (1971). The nonanomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Linguistics 4, 47-68.; Fromkin (1973). Speech errors as linguistic evidence. The Hauge: Mouton.; Fromkin, V. & Ratner, N. B. (1998). Speech production. In J. B. Gleason & N. B. Ratner (Eds.), Psycholinguistics (pp. 309-346). California: Wadsworth.; Garrett, M. F. (1980). Levels of processing in sentence production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Speech production (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.; Garrett, M. F. (1988). Processes in language production. In F. J. Newmeyer (Ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey III. Language: Psychological and biological aspects (pp.69-96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.; Gordon, Jean K. (2002). Phonological neighborhood effects in aphasic speech errors: spontaneous and structured contexts. Brain and Language, 82, 113-145.; Hockett, C. F. (1967). Where the tongue slips, there slip I. In V. Fromkin (Ed.), Speech errors as linguistic evidence (pp.93-119). Netherlands: Mouton, the Hague.; Hotopf, W. H. N. (1980). Semantic similarity as a factor in whole-word slips of the tongue. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand, 97–109. New York: Academic Press.; Huang, C. T. James, Li, Y.-H. Audrey & Li, Y. (2008). The syntax of Chinese. Cambridge University Press.; Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intension to articulation. Cambridge: MIT Press.; Levelt, W. J. M. (ed.) (1993). Lexical Access in Speech Production. UK: Blackwell Publisher.; Levelt, W. J. M. (1993). Accessing Words in Speech Production: Stages, Processes and Representations. Lexical Access in Speech Production, 1-22.; Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75.; Levitt, A.G. & Healy, A.F. (1985). The roles of phonese frequency, similarity, and availability in the experimental elicitation of speech errors. Journal of Memory and Language 24, 717-733.; Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: the Neighborhood Activation Model. Ear and Hearing, 1–36.; Mackay, D. G. (1970). Spoonerisms: The structure of errors in the serial order of speech. Neuropsychologia 8, 323-350.; Mackay, D. G. (1972). The structure of wrds and syllables: Evidence from errors in speech. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 210-227.; Maddieson, Ian (1986). The size and structural of phonological inventories: analysis of UPSID. In Ohala, John & Jeri Jaeger (eds), Experimental Phonology. Academic Press.; Martin, N., Gagnon, D. A., Schwartz, M. F., Dell, G. S. & Saffran, E. M. (1996). Phonological facilitation of semantic errors in normal and aphasic speakers. Language and Cognitive Processes 11 (3), 257-282.; Martin, N., & Ayala, J. (2004). Measurements of auditoryverbal STM in aphasia: effects of task, item and word processing impairment. Brain and Language, 8, 464–483.; McClelland & Rumelhart (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review 88 (5), 375-407.; Meringer, R. & Mayer, K. (1895). Versprechen und Verlesen: eine psychologisch-linguistische Studie. Stuttgart: Goschense Verlagsbuchhandlung.; Meyer, A. S. (1990). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The encoding of successive syllables of a word. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 524-545.; Meyer, A. S. (1991). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 69-89.; Nooteboom, S.G. (1969). The tongue slips into patterns. Leyden Studies in Linguistics and Phonetics. The Hague: Mouton.; Oppenheim, G. M. & Dell, G. S. (2008). Inner speech slips exhibit lexical bias, but not the phonemic similarity effect. Cognition, 106, 528-537.; Packard, J. L. (1986). Tone Production deficits in nonfluent aphasic Chinese speech, Brain and Language, 29, 212-223.; Rapp, D. N. & Samuel, A.G. (2002). A reason to rhyme: Phonological and semantic influences on lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 28 (3), 564-571.; Roelofs, A. (1996). Computationa; models of lemma retrieval. In T. Dijkstra & K. de Smedt (Eds.) Computational Psycholinguistics: AI and connectionist mode4ls of human language processing (pp.308-327). Taylor & Francis.; Roelofs, A. (1997) The WEAVER model of word-form encoding in speech production.; Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A.S. (1998). Metrical structure in planning the production of spoken words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 922–939.; Schwartz, M. F. & Dell, G. S. & Martin, N. (2004). Testing the interactive two-step model of lexical access: Part I. picture naming, Brain and Language, 91, 71-72.; Sevald, C. A., Dell, G. S., &Cole, J. (1995). Syllable structure in speech production: Are syllable chunks or schemas? Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 807-820.; Shattuck-Hufnagel (1979). Speech errors as evidence for a serial ordering mechanism in sentence production. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walkers (Eds.), Sentence processing (pp.295-342). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Assiciates.; Shattuck-Hufnagel (1986). The representation of phonologoical information during speech production planning: Evidence from vowel errors in spontaneous speech. Phonology Yearbook 3, 117-149.; Shattuck-Hufnagel (1987). The role of word onset consonants in speech production planning: New evidence from speech error patterns. In E. Keller & M. Gopnik (Eds.), Motor and sensory processing in language (pp. 17-51). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.; Schwartz, M. F. (1994). Disordered Speech Production in Aphasic and Normal Speakers. Brain and Language, 47, 52-88.; Sokolov, A. N. (1972). Inner speech and thought. New York: Plenum.; Stemberger, J. P. (1983). Speech errors and theoretical phonology: A review. Bloomington: Indiana Linguistics Club.; Stemberger, J. P. (1984). Structural Errors in Normal and Agrammatic Speech. Cognitve Neuropsychology, 1 (4), 281-313.; Stemberger, J. P. (1985). An interactive activation model of language production. In W. E. Andrew (Ed.), Progress in the psychology of language (pp. 143-186). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd., Publishers.; Stemberger, J. P. (1989). Speech errors in early child language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 28( 2), 164-188.; Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.; Vitevitch, M. S. (1997). The neighborhood characteristics of malapropisms. Language and Speech, 40, 211–228.; Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (1998). When words compete: levels of processing in perception of spoken words. Psychological Science, 9, 325–329.; Vitevitch, M. S. (2002, July). The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on speech production. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 28(4), 735–747.; Vitevitch, M. S., Armbruster, J., & Chu, S. (2004). Sublexical and lexical representations in speech production: effects of phonotactic probability and onset density. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 30(2), 514–529.; Vitevitch, M. S., & Sommers, M. S. (2003). The facilitative influence of phonological similarity and neighborhood frequency in speech production in younger and older adults. Memory and Cognition, 31, 491–504.; Vitevitch, M. S., & Stamer, M. K. (2006). The curious case of competition in Spanish speech production. Language & Cognitive Processes, 21(6), 760–770.; Wan, I. P. & Jeri J. (1998) Speexh errors and the representation of tone in Mandarin Chinese. Phonology, 15, 417-461.; Wan, I. P. (1999). Mandarin phonology: Evidence from speech errors. Ph D. Dissertation.; Wan, I. P. (2007). Aphonological Investigation in Speech Errors and Aphasic Speech in Mandarin. Taipei: Crane.; Wells, R. (1951). Predicting slips of the tongue. Yale Scientific Magazine, 3, 9-30.; G0097555009; http://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw//handle/140.119/52705

  19. 19
    Dissertation/ Thesis

    المؤلفون: 陳慧盈, Chen,Huei ying

    المساهمون: 萬依萍, Wan,I Ping

  20. 20